The Role of Autism in Polynesian Navigation
The past 40 years has seen a remarkable renaissance of Polynesian
cultural awareness, with rediscovery and celebration of ancient skills and
practices. Polynesians are taking pride in their heritage - particularly the seafaring
skills that made settlement of the Pacific islands possible. The navigation techniques developed and
practiced by these island people were distinctly different from those developed
by Europeans. They were originally
characterized as primitive by western anthropologists, but we now realize they
were in many ways more sophisticated than methods developed in the west.
Polynesian navigators are called wayfinders, and their craft
is called wayfinding. Western researchers have studied their techniques over
the past 50 years. Their efficacy is firmly established; what’s missing is a
study of the intellectual requirements of wayfinding. This is significant because European
navigators rely on measurements with instruments and pen and paper
calculation. Thanks to tools and method,
most people can learn to do it. Wayfinding
does not use tools or pen and paper; it is all “in the head,” and for that
reason it is more challenging.
Wayfinders are consequently much less common. This essay considers the cognitive issues and
explores a possible relationship between those voyagers and autism.
Autism is a neurological difference that was first
recognized in people who showed profound disability but at the same time,
flashes of intellectual exceptionality. Autism has been recognized since the 1930s but
it has undergone a renaissance of its own in terms of understanding in the past
50 years. At first, autism was only
diagnosed in people with very severe disability. Then in the 1990s the diagnostic criteria
were broadened. The phrase
“communication disability” that once meant “nonverbal person” now refers to
anything from that to inability to understand facial expressions in an
otherwise articulate person. With that
change, far more autistic people were recognized, especially kids. At the same time, the perception of autism
changed. Where it was seen as a crushing disability, autism is now viewed on a
continuum. There are extremely disabled autistic
people, but the autism spectrum also encompasses some gifted individuals.
The mental gymnastics performed by wayfinders appear to
match capabilities bright autistic people are known for, and excel at. At the same time, the wayfinding job and its social
context seems to be one where autistic disabilities would be minimized. The closer a knowledgeable (with respect to
autism) person looks, the better the fit appears to be.
Anthropologists who are accustomed to seeing autism through
the lens of disability might initially doubt the connection because wayfinders
have traditionally been important figures in island cultures. They are not
socially isolated as the disability model might predict. Yet that does not rule out their being
autistic. There are many autistic
leaders in western society.
Profoundly disabled autistics make less than one-half percent
of the human population. The number of
people who have some autistic traits without total disability is much
larger. The autism spectrum – the term for
all autistic people – includes 1.5% to 2% of humanity. Adding in people who have some autistic
traits but not enough for formal diagnosis yields what researchers call the
“broad autism phenotype,” which may total 5% of the population. This essay explores
some of their unique attributes and why autistics might have been drawn to
wayfinding.
We begin by looking at the wayfinding task, and asking a
question: what kind of person sets sail in a vessel, with no navigational
instruments, into the vastness of the South Pacific, and travels 2,500 miles on
winds and currents to reach a destination island that is just 50 miles wide in
an otherwise open sea? Wayfinders have
extraordinary sensory and calculating abilities – two areas of common strength
in bright autistic people.
We understand that different types of people are best at
certain jobs. Building houses requires
manual skills - dexterity, and brawn. Assembling
electronic devices calls for precise coordination. A few jobs – like code
breaking or software engineering – require special cognitive skills. Some jobs with
unusual cognitive demands may be particularly suited to autistic people. A
western example of that might be a computer science or math department at a
university, where people joke that “half the faculty are on the autism
spectrum,” with more than a grain of truth to the statement. Hans Asperger –
one of the doctors who originally characterized autism – noted the connection
between autism and certain technical and creative pursuits in the 1930s.
When thinking of a cognitively demanding job you might
imagine engineering or science. Those
are good examples, but they are fairly new to humanity in the form we see
today. Wayfinding is an example of a job
that was cognitively demanding in a less technological society, such as existed
in the South Pacific.
The job is not physically strenuous, though it does require stamina,
as the navigator must be awake and alert for most of a voyage. He/she must
remember a vast body of data about the movements of stars, sun and moon. The wayfinder must sense direction and
currents from the feel of swells acting upon his vessel, and be attuned to clues
in the environment, like birds showing the direction to land. Finally the wayfinder must integrate
observations with memory, and extrapolate from what’s known of places
previously visited to what is anticipated for the destination. An in-depth description
of wayfinding can be found in David Lewis’s 1972 book, We the Navigators.
The complexity of wayfinding is easily underestimated,
particularly compared to western navigation.
Anyone observing the navigator aboard a modern merchant ship sees a
person surrounded by high-tech gear – satellite navigation, depth finders,
radar and more. The array of equipment
fairly screams out “complex task!” A
Pacific wayfinder, in comparison, has nothing in his/her hands, and no tools or
aids in sight. They simply stands on the ship’s deck and observe, occasionally
giving directions to the helmsman. There
is no observable evidence of the calculations taking place in the mind.
Until recently, non-sailing anthropologists doubted early
wayfinders had the ability to deliberately sail open canoes thousands of miles
between islands like Tahiti, New Zealand, and Hawaii. After all, European navigators didn’t learn
to calculate longitude (position on an east-west axis) until the 18th
century, and they thought that was essential to precise navigation, especially
when it came to finding islands.
Prior to developing the ability to calculate longitude
European sailors were left with latitude (north-south orientation) as their
only measure of position. Early
navigators had little understanding of ocean currents or other factors that
might affect their course. Luckily when
Europeans ventured west they had two huge continents as targets. No matter what direction he sailed, an early
European navigator would fetch up somewhere on the shores of North or South
America.
The Pacific Ocean presented a rather different problem.
There, tiny islands were scattered over millions of square miles of deep
trackless ocean. A Pacific navigator needs
to hold a course much more precisely to avoid missing his targets. That was a problem European navigators of the
last millennium did not solve until the invention of the chronometer, so
anthropologists assumed the Polynesians – who did not have chronometers – must
never have solved it for themselves.
In a classic display of ethnocentricity some anthropologists
concluded Polynesian sailors must have reached distant destinations only by lucky
accident. Their arguments were buttressed with stories of shipwrecked European
navigators. However studies showed winds
and currents make it impossible for a boat to drift from Tahiti to Hawaii yet
legends describe voyaging between the two islands. Polynesians insisted the
settlement of their islands was purposeful, not accidental.
Westerners also doubted the ability of craft described as
“canoes” to cross vast distances of open sea.
That notion was largely founded on misunderstanding – a Polynesian
voyaging canoe has nothing in common with the car-top craft of the same name
that Americans know and love. In fact,
Polynesian voyaging canoes have much more in common with the sophisticated
twin-hull sailing yachts that routinely cross the oceans today, and in many
ways they are even more rugged and seaworthy.
A group of Hawaiians set out to prove the hypothesis that
their boatbuilding and navigation skills were sufficient to have settled the
vast Pacific. In 1975 they launched the
first Hawaiian voyaging canoe to set sail in 600 years – the Hōkūle‘a. Built in
the pattern of the voyaging canoes of the last millennium, Hōkūle‘a proved to
be a remarkably capable craft. When
navigated by traditional methods of star and current observation, Hōkūle‘a
crossed 2,500 miles of open sea between Hawaii and Tahiti to make landfall with
as much accuracy as any western craft in the pre-GPS era. The navigator for that trip was Mau Piailug,
a wayfinder from the island of Satawal.
His apprentice was Hawaiian Nainoa Thompson, who continues as a
wayfinder today and leads the Polynesian Voyaging Society. Will Kyselka’s 1987 book An Ocean in Mind describes these voyages and the individuals who
made them.
Piailug, Thompson, and the wayfinders they trained show that
accurate navigation is possible without the use of any modern instruments. The mind and hand alone are sufficiently
powerful tools, given correct training and suitable cognitive powers. The culture of Polynesian navigation that has
been passed on from master to apprentice is once again enjoying resurgence
thanks to groups like the Polynesian Voyaging Society.
When we consider that traditional navigation is a skill
Polynesian cultures have guarded and preserved for over 2,000 years we might ask
if there is anything special about the individuals chosen to become navigators.
I began to wonder about that when I
learned of the cognitive complexity of Polynesian navigation, particularly the
idea it is “all in the mind.” The
difference between that and European-style navigation is striking given that
the latter relies on specialized tools and pen and paper calculation, where the
Polynesian system uses no tools at all yet it produces an equally functional
result.
The early Polynesian people had more of a seafaring culture
than what is retained today. Young
navigators apprenticed themselves to master navigators and learned the stars,
swells, and currents through observation on long ocean voyages. Wayfinder accounts of the last century
suggest that apprenticeship started as early as five years of age. Navigators
often ran in families, with older men training promising sons or nephews. That process has been largely lost with the
disappearance of traditional long distance Polynesian sailing craft, though
there is a recent move to resurrect it.
Modern-day Polynesian navigators train for voyages by
looking at the stars and “learning the sky.” They make mental maps of the sky
and gain understanding of the relationship between time, one’s position on
earth, and the stars. This gives them a
deeper understanding of the relationship of the sky to one’s position on earth
than that held by European sailors of the last 1,000 years and it is key to
their success. In addition to the stars
wayfinders learn to read ocean swells and currents and interpret countless
other clues that point the way to land over the horizon.
Man’s ability to acquire and use this knowledge is a
remarkable display of human cognition. When
westerners learn navigation, they do so within a complex framework of map
reading and advanced mathematics to calculate latitude and longitude. For westerners, navigation also relies on
precise timekeeping – hence the importance of the chronometer in our
culture. Pacific navigators do not use
those tools. Their navigation is more
elemental. Pacific islanders find their position accurately (with respect to
their home and destination islands) without knowledge of western math or
celestial mechanics. They grasp and
manipulate the complex concepts of position fixing instinctively.
A westerner can learn basic instrument navigation in a few
weeks. However most don’t even do that –
they rely on navigation devices to show the way with no training at all. The downside of that is, they are totally
lost if the tools fail. Polynesian
wayfinders spend a lifetime training their minds but they are then free of
dependence on tools or technology. The more we learn about wayfinding the more
respect we can have for the few people who master its challenges. To put their achievements in western terms,
master wayfinders are in many respects Olympians of the mind.
After studying all the requirements, it appears that certain
autistic people – including those of the broader autism phenotype – are
uniquely suited to the cognitive demands of wayfinding. Very few non-autistic people
can gaze at the sky with enough intensity and focus to burn an accurate map
into their heads, particularly as the map moves when the navigator changes
position, time or season. Yet anyone who
works with autistic people would look at that challenge and see an autistic
strength.
That’s not the only hint of autism in wayfinding. In the accounts of Lewis and others, wayfinders
exhibit what may be other traits of autism, such as lack of social awareness. Autistic
people tend to have some degree of social disability, either from challenges
with language or blindness to other social cues. Polynesian sailing vessels had small, tightly
knit crews, where such disabilities would be minimized by familiarity. A Polynesian wayfinder would be less disabled
by autism in his job than he might be in most traditional western workplaces.
What’s most important to the wayfinding role are some of the
gifts certain autistic people have.
These gifts are part and parcel of autism because they have the same
neurological roots as the disability aspects; the two go hand in hand. Autism is not widely recognized among
present-day native Hawaiians and Polynesians. Yet studies suggest it should be
just as common there as elsewhere in the world.
In reading accounts of present-day Polynesian navigators, I see many
signs of the broad autism phenotype.
Some of the autistic traits that can be seen in descriptions
of Polynesian navigators are:
·
Social isolation – wayfinders are often
portrayed in text as loners or strongly independent. Being independent is not
itself diagnostic of autism, but independence and aloneness are common traits for
autistic people.
·
Anecdotes of wayfinders often present them as
socially inept or unaware. Gaffes were attributed to culture (i.e.; being “from
Satawal” or another distant island) but they may just as well have been due to
autistic social disability;
·
Accounts of wayfinders describe multiple
generations of navigators, who were trained from early childhood, and who then
trained the next generation. Autism has
a strong inherited component, so the cognitive abilities needed for wayfinding
would likely be passed on in a family line.
·
In his descriptions of wayfinders, Lewis
described men who were very tied to routines.
He described several instances of distress when routines were broken.
Restricted interests, love of routine, and distress when routine is broken are
diagnostic traits of autism.
·
Most descriptions of wayfinders describe them as
very focused on their craft. Extreme
focus and extraordinary powers of concentration are common in autistic people.
·
Video of navigators like Mau Piailug (See
maupiailugsociety videos on youtube) shows scenes an autism therapist would
describe as “very autistic.” Behavioral
examples include limited facial expressions; animation of the bottom of the face
but not the top; gaze at the floor rather than other people; and cadence and
pattern of speech. While no one would
propose to render an autism diagnosis from a short series of videos, it is more
possible to recognize the broader autism phenotype.
·
Wayfinders need an ability to sense subtle clues
in the environment, like the way ocean swells feel under the boat. Autistic people are prone to extraordinary
sensory sensitivities, and they are typically good at recognizing patterns or
deviations from them (like the way swells feel.) In his accounts of voyaging with Piailug,
Thompson describes how the older man could sense and feel things invisible to
him. That could well be an example of
superior sensitivity that might be disabling in some contexts on shore but was
a great gift at sea.
·
Wayfinders rely on knowledge and logic as
opposed to emotion. Lewis makes that point when quoting wayfinders, who assure
him their craft is based on knowledge and not superstition or belief. A preference for logic over emotion is
suggestive of autism.
·
A strong ability to systematize is essential to
wayfinding. Examples are judging position from the star map overhead and the
classification and interpretation of ocean swells and other evidence relevant
to position and course. Autistic people
tend to be strong systematizers;
·
Given the number of stars one can see in the
sky, a person’s ability to memorize the position data that is revealed by
patterns of stars aligning or setting must be extraordinary. Thompson says he uses several hundred stars
for navigation. Such exceptionality is extremely rare in the general population
but somewhat common among autistics.
·
Exceptional calendar calculating skills are very
useful for manipulating and evaluating celestial maps in the head. Autistics are the only group of people known
to commonly possess calendar calculating skills. Psychiatrist Darold Treffert
has suggested more than 6% of autistics have extraordinary calendar skills.
·
Exceptional visual memory is necessary to
acquire the star maps needed for navigation, and that too is common in
autistics but rare in the general population.
·
Finally, a wayfinder needs the ability to
transform complex visual images in the mind (i.e. sophisticated thinking in
pictures.)
While none of these skills are individually diagnostic of autism
they are – when taken together – strongly suggestive that a person fitting this
wayfinder description is part of the broader phenotype, and may be autistic. Of course, not every autistic person is a
potential wayfinder. The required ability set is probably very rare in the
population but to the extent it exists at all, it will be found among members
of the autism community.
Some researchers refer to autistic people as “nature’s
engineers” because they can learn complex computational skills on their own,
without the need for schools or modern teaching practices. Exceptional focus and concentration are
autistic traits, and in this case they may have helped Polynesian autistics to
acquire the skills to design and then navigate their vessels, thereby
facilitating the original settlement of the Pacific islands.
The question of autism in Polynesian navigators actually
raises another question – Does a western diagnostic label that we associate
with disability have any relevance when applied to a wayfinder in the South
Pacific? That question struck me as I
watched old video of Mau Piailug, who died in 2010. When I watched him in the videos I saw many
signs of the broad autism phenotype in his speech, his expressions, and his
behavior. Yet he was a respected leader
in his community; there is no evidence that anyone perceived him as disabled in
his lifetime. To apply a disability
diagnosis now from afar would strike many people as disrespectful and wrong.
A significant number of Polynesian wayfinders may have been
autistic throughout the years. We have
no way to know. The fact is, autism per se had nothing to do with their finding
their profession. They were chosen for their ancestry or their behaviors – both
of which might suggest “autistic” to us but suggested “navigator material” to
the Polynesians. It’s worth pondering
which worldview is more personally empowering.
On a pacific island world, an autism diagnosis has no
meaning. The place it has meaning is in
our hi-tech western world. It’s here
that autistics are disabled, and seeking explanation and insight. For an autistic teen in a modern-day Hawaiian
school, the idea that a great wayfinder like Piailug may be “autistic like me”
is very empowering. What it shows is
that a class of people who are mostly disabled and less capable in our society
can be exceptional in other circumstances and cultures.
It’s a fascinating idea.
John Elder Robison
Neurodiversity Scholar,
The College of William & Mary
Williamsburg, VA
Visiting Professor of Practice, Education
Bay Path University
Longmeadow, MA
John Elder Robison is an autistic adult who studies the role
of autistic people in society. He
navigates a small boat on inland rivers near his home in Western Massachusetts. He is the NY Times bestselling author of four
books on life with autism: Look Me in the Eye, Be Different, Raising
Cubby, and Switched On.
Comments
Great article, it gives me a lot to think about. I have been looking into and working on developing my right-brain capability, as you know autism is characterized by extreme left-brain systermizing.
All of the attributes and traits described here for Wayfinding strike me as not just very right-brained activities, but activities that require a firm balance in both left and right hemispheres. Hemispheric synchronization. It, however, appears "extraordinary" to us because global society as a while is strongly left-brained, so any foray into right-brain or hemispheric synchronization appears in such a strange/mythic/unusual way.
As part of my own journey into my right-brain side, I have developed intuitive abilities that are so far in range that it would be exhaustive to describe. However, one of these new abilities is similar to Wayfinding. I am a person who has gotten lost hundreds of times in my life and often found that trying to navigate myself ends badly. Recently, I have been taking spontaneous trips either into natural areas or into unknown urban areas, where I would normally be very lost and be unable to keep my schedules and day in order. For many months now I have been able to navigate myself solely through instinct and not thinking about the direction or time much at all. I am even able to keep specific deadlines whilst relying on pure instinct (such as the last bus home). I am able to navigate myself to specific places or even specific items that I might need, just as firewood. This is all purely on right-brain instinct and little to no left-brain tracking.
I am normally very sceptical of an autism label on people from the past, as I think what you are describing in this article isn't so much autism, but simple an expression of a very broad autism phenotype that is expressed because of the specific nature of the Wayfinding work (possibly due to hemispheric synchronization). I think this can express as autism today in our kind of global society which is suffering from a severe case of oversystemization.
I should point out that I think you neglect the importance of neuroplasticity and how the human psyche can vary throughout time and place. The people in that distant past were almost certainly more rooted in their right-brain, therefore able to casually perform feats that would seem fantastical to most people today.
I met Dr Felipe Fregni and I was invited to his lab for a summer non invasive bain stimulation training program in 2012. When I came back I was convinced of the potential therapetic uses of TMS in diverse neuropshyquiatric pathologies. I am now working with autism and I have seen impressive results. Here our budget for research is very limited and nobody is interested in this field for research. But there are a lot of patients demanding treatment for this condition and we are offering it. We know what we are doing and we are also confident because our job is based on ethics, knowledge and responsability. Considering this, I find unfair your judgement of other researchers worldwide (https://youtu.be/p-kY9zBXTiQ), if They are not working at Dr Pascual Leone Lab. We do need support, there are a lot of families waiting for this treatment to be accesible for children. Se admire hoy but we are also surprised about the very pessimistic message, since you are a reference for us as researchers and families worldwide.
Thank you.
Judy (An Autism Observer)
shows humbleness to those they speak to by not challenging with direct eye contact. It is a sign of respect. This is the problem for a lot of their children as they do not speak up in class or challenge a teachers knowledge for fear of disrespecting them. Just thought I would add these points for your consideration. Thanks for the opportunity to add another perspective. Corine.